Jonathan
Leake, the Science Editor for the Sunday Times, reports today that “women
appear to have won a decisive victory in the battle of the sexes. Psychologists
have found female IQ scores have risen above men for the first time. The
findings have been made by James Flynn, a world-renowned authority on IQ tests.
‘In the last 100 years the IQ scores of both men and women have risen but women’s
have risen faster’ said Flynn. This is a consequence of modernity. The complexity
of the modern world is making our brains adapt and raising our IQ.” But does IQ
translate into performance?
Flynn
gives one possible explanation as “women’s lives have become more demanding as
they multitask between raising a family and doing a job. Another is that women
have a slightly higher potential intelligence than men and are only now
realising it”.
IQ
scores are used as predictors of educational achievement, special needs, job
performance and income. They are also used to study IQ distributions in
populations and the correlations between IQ and other variables. The average IQ
scores for many populations have been rising at an average rate of three points
per decade since the early 20th century, a phenomenon called the Flynn effect.
It is disputed whether these changes in scores reflect real changes in
intellectual abilities.
Whether
IQ tests are an accurate measure of intelligence is debated. It is difficult to
define what constitutes intelligence; instead it may be the case that IQ
represents a type of intelligence.
According
to Frank Schmidt and John Hunter, "for hiring employees without previous
experience in the job the most valid predictor of future performance is general
mental ability." The validity of IQ as a predictor of job performance is
above zero for all work studied to date, but varies with the type of job and
across different studies, ranging from 0.2 to 0.6. The correlations were higher
when the unreliability of measurement methods was controlled for. While IQ is
more strongly correlated with reasoning and less so with motor function,
IQ-test scores predict performance ratings in all occupations.
That
said, for highly qualified activities (research, management) low IQ scores are
more likely to be a barrier to adequate performance, whereas for
minimally-skilled activities, athletic strength (manual strength, speed,
stamina, and coordination) are more likely to influence performance. It is
largely mediated through the quicker acquisition of job-relevant knowledge that
IQ predicts job performance.
In
establishing a causal direction to the link between IQ and work performance,
longitudinal studies by Watkins and others suggest that IQ exerts a causal
influence on future academic achievement, whereas academic achievement does not
substantially influence future IQ scores. Treena Eileen Rohde and Lee Anne
Thompson write that general cognitive ability but not specific ability scores
predict academic achievement, with the exception that processing speed and
spatial ability predict performance on the SAT math beyond the effect of
general cognitive ability.
Eliza
Byington and Will Felps highlight how, “over the past century, numerous studies
have documented the link between cognitive assessment scores and employee
performance (see Schmidt & Hunter, 1992, 1998, 2004 for overviews). The
strength of these findings has made intelligence “the most important trait or construct
in all of psychology, and the most ‘successful’ trait in applied psychology”
(Schmidt &Hunter, 1986). Few areas of scholarship produce such unequivocal
findings and prescriptions for managers. In a chapter advocating the principle
that employers should “select on intelligence,”Schmidt and Hunter state:
“Intelligence is the major determinant of job performance, and therefore hiring
people based on intelligence leads to marked improvements in job performance
–improvements that have high economic value to the firm” (2000: 3). The
assertion that intelligence leads to job performance is echoed in the titles of
recent articles in which management researchers describe their findings:
“Intelligence is the best predictor of job performance” (Ree & Earles,
1992), “The role of general cognitive ability and job performance: Why there
cannot be a debate” (Schmidt, 2002), and “Predicting job performance: Not much more
than g” (Ree, Earles, & Teachout, 1994). Building on this empirical
foundation, researchers have gone so far as to provide methods by which
managers and HR professionals can calculate the positive economic value of
employing IQ-based selection in their organizations (e.g. Rauschenberger &
Schmidt, 1987; Schmidt, Hunter, McKenzie, & Muldrow, 1979; Hunter
&Schmidt, 1996; Schmidt & Hunter 1998).
The
industrial psychology literature has also reached a consensus on the
explanation for the strong IQ – job performance relationship; namely, that individuals
who are more intelligent (as measured by IQ scores) can learn job relevant
knowledge faster and better, resulting in improved job performance (e.g. Ree,
Carretta, & Teachout, 1995;Hunter, 1986)” (p.176).
However
Byington and Felps weren’t totally convinced that the relationship between IQ
and performance was a simple as past research had made it, stating “we suggest
that the relationship between IQ and job performance is strongest in contexts
where institutional architects have already adopted this edict, and in so
doing, helped create its validity” – a bit like a self-fulfilling prophecy.
Tomas
Chamorro-Premuzic probably sums it up best when he mentions that “there is a
clear discrepancy between the scientific research evidence and what laypeople,
and even some professional practitioners (HR departments, managers, recruiters,
etc.) tend to believe. Thus scientists concluded long time ago that IQ is the
most powerful psychological predictor of job performance, whereas laypeople
tend to think of IQ as something that is useful only when it comes to
predicting how well you do in a maths or general knowledge quiz - indeed,
people with extremely high IQs are almost always portrayed as being geeky,
weird and socially inept (an urban legend that has made Goleman, Gardner, and
Sternberg rich and famous)”.
References
Byington,
E and Felps, W. (2010). Why do IQ scores predict job performance? An
alternative, sociological explanation. Research in Organizational Behavior, Vol.
30, p.175-202.
Chamorro-Premuzic,
T. (2011). How Many Successful Entrepreneurs Would Fail an IQ Test? Psychology
Today.
Leake,
J. (2012). Women really are cleverer. The Sunday Times, 15th July,
p.1.
No comments:
Post a Comment